Wednesday, October 04, 2023

 

Female animals may learn mate preferences based on what sets other females’ choices apart from the crowd


Mathematical model suggests females learn to prefer rare traits that distinguish successful males


Peer-Reviewed Publication

PLOS

Female animals may learn mate preferences based on what sets other females’ choices apart from the crowd 

IMAGE: BY FOCUSING ON THE CONTEXT OF FEMALE MATE CHOICES, THE INFERRED ATTRACTIVENESS HYPOTHESIS PROVIDES A NEW PERSPECTIVE ON HOW SEXUAL SELECTION MAY FUNCTION. ARTIST’S RENDERING OF MATE CHOICE BY A PEAHEN. view more 

CREDIT: COMPOSITE IMAGE BY EMILY DUVAL AND ELLIOT SCHUNKE, ORIGINAL IMAGES BY JEAN VAN DER MEULEN AND SALLY WYNN (PLATINUMPORTFOLIO) (CC-BY 4.0, HTTPS://CREATIVECOMMONS.ORG/LICENSES/BY/4.0/)




Females may infer what makes a male attractive by observing the choices of more experienced females, and the context of those choices matters, according to a mathematical model publishing October 3rd in the open access journal PLOS Biology. Rather than simply copying their peers, females might learn to prefer rare traits that set successful males apart from others, Emily DuVal at Florida State University, US, and colleagues report.

Sexual selection — where traits become more common because of their attractiveness to the opposite sex — can produce strange and elaborate characteristics, such as huge antlers, bright plumage, and flamboyant courtship dances. However, exactly why females prefer certain traits over others is poorly understood. Female preferences in a given population often change across generations, and sometimes preferences differ among individuals within one population. Existing theories suggest that females prefer traits indicating genetic quality; that male traits and female preferences are linked in a positive feedback loop; or that females’ senses bias them towards certain traits. But no theory fully explains the variety of traits and preferences seen in nature.

To address this, researchers developed a mathematical model in which females learn which traits are attractive by watching others. In the model, young females observe the mate choices of adult females and learn to prefer traits that distinguish the chosen male from other males. In other words, females learned to prefer the rarest trait of a successful male, but this trait was not necessarily what the observed female was really choosing. Over several generations, female preferences caused rare male traits to become more common, which then made them less attractive. This helped to maintain variation in male traits, rather than a single attractive trait outcompeting the others.

The results of this mathematical model are consistent with several features of sexual selection in nature, such as rapid evolutionary changes, and the persistence of variation in male traits and female preferences. Animals use social information to make decisions in many contexts. Inferring the attractiveness of potential mates may be an extension of this general tendency, the authors say.

DuVal and her co-authors add, “While scientists have known for a long time that females can copy each other’s choice of mates, no one has previously considered that these copying females aren’t mind-readers.  When we considered that females can make mistakes in identifying what traits others find attractive, we found this produces patterns that have long puzzled biologists, for example maintaining variety in male traits and female preferences over time.”

#####

In your coverage, please use this URL to provide access to the freely available paper in PLOS Biologyhttp://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/article?id=10.1371/journal.pbio.3002269  

Citation: DuVal EH, Fitzpatrick CL, Hobson EA, Servedio MR (2023) Inferred Attractiveness: A generalized mechanism for sexual selection that can maintain variation in traits and preferences over time. PLoS Biol 21(10): e3002269. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002269

Author Countries: United States

Funding: Financial support during preparation of this work was provided by National Science Foundation Integrative Organismal Systems award 1453408, Division of Biological Infrastructure award 1457541, and Division of Environmental Biology 2243423 (to EHD), National Institutes of Health T32 HD049336 (to CLF), National Science Foundation Integrative Organismal Systems award 2015932 and The Santa Fe Institute (to EAH), and National Science Foundation Division of Environmental Biology award 1939290 (to MRS). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

No comments:

Post a Comment