Tuesday, October 26, 2021

That primate’s got rhythm!


Peer-Reviewed Publication

MAX PLANCK INSTITUTE FOR PSYCHOLINGUISTICS

Researchers from the universities of Turin, Lyon/Saint-Étienne and the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics in Nijmegen studied indris, the ‘singing primates’ from Madagascar 

VIDEO: RESEARCHERS FROM THE UNIVERSITIES OF TURIN, LYON/SAINT-ÉTIENNE AND THE MAX PLANCK INSTITUTE FOR PSYCHOLINGUISTICS IN NIJMEGEN STUDIED INDRIS, THE ‘SINGING PRIMATES’ FROM MADAGASCAR view more 

CREDIT: ANDREA RAVIGNANI

Songbirds share the human sense of rhythm, but it is a rare trait in non-human mammals. An international research team led by senior investigators Marco Gamba from the University of Turin and MPI’s Andrea Ravignani set out to look for musical abilities in primates. “There is longstanding interest in understanding how human musicality evolved, but musicality is not restricted to humans”, says Ravignani. “Looking for musical features in other species allows us to build an ‘evolutionary tree’ of musical traits, and understand how rhythm capacities originated and evolved in humans.”

To find out whether non-human mammals have a sense of rhythm, the team decided to study one of the few ‘singing’ primates, the critically endangered lemur Indri indri. The researchers wanted to know whether indri songs have categorical rhythm, a ‘rhythmic universal’ found across human musical cultures. Rhythm is categorical when intervals between sounds have exactly the same duration (1:1 rhythm) or doubled duration (1:2 rhythm). This type of rhythm makes a song easily recognisable, even if it is sung at different speeds. Would indri songs show this “uniquely human” rhythm?

CAPTION

Indri songs recorded in the wild have rhythmic categories similar to those found in human music.

CREDIT

Filippo Carugati

Ritardando in the rainforest

Over a period of twelve years, the researchers from Turin visited the rainforest of Madagascar to collaborate with a local primate study group. The investigators recorded songs from twenty indri groups (39 animals), living in their natural habitat. Members of an indri family group tend to sing together, in harmonised duets and choruses. The team found that indri songs had the classic rhythmic categories (both 1:1 and 1:2), as well as the typical ‘ritardando’ or slowing down found in several musical traditions. Male and female songs had a different tempo but showed the same rhythm.

According to first author Chiara de Gregorio and her colleagues, this is the first evidence of a ‘rhythmic universal’ in a non-human mammal. But why should another primate produce categorical ‘music-like’ rhythms? The ability may have evolved independently among ‘singing’ species, as the last common ancestor between humans and indri lived 77.5 million years ago. Rhythm may make it easier to produce and process songs, or even to learn them.

CAPTION

Finding common musical traits across species may shed light on the biology and evolution of rhythm and music.

CREDIT

Filippo Carugati


Endangered species

“Categorical rhythms are just one of the six universals that have been identified so far”, explains Ravignani. “We would like to look for evidence of others, including an underlying ‘repetitive’ beat and a hierarchical organisation of beats—in indri and other species.” The authors encourage other researchers to gather data on indri and other endangered species, “before it is too late to witness their breath-taking singing displays.”

Aquatic fungus has already wiped amphibians off the map and now threatens survival of terrestrial frogs


A study detected unprecedented mortality in the Atlantic Rainforest among tiny frogs that live on land, with signs of infection by chytrid fungus. The episode coincided with an atypical period of drought.

Peer-Reviewed Publication

FUNDAÇÃO DE AMPARO À PESQUISA DO ESTADO DE SÃO PAULO

Mortality in the Atlantic Rainforest among tiny frogs 

IMAGE: THE EPISODE COINCIDED WITH AN ATYPICAL PERIOD OF DROUGHT, WHICH MAY HAVE FORCED THE ANIMALS TO SEEK WATER IN STREAMS WHERE THE PATHOGEN IS ABUNDANT view more 

CREDIT: DIEGO MOURA-CAMPOS/UNICAMP

A water-borne fungus that has led to the extinction of several species of amphibians that spend all or part of their life cycle in water is also threatening terrestrial amphibians. In Brazil, researchers supported by FAPESP detected unprecedented mortality among a genus of tiny frogs known as pumpkin toadlets that live in the Atlantic Rainforest far from any aquatic environments. The animals were severely infected by chytrid fungus (Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis), which causes chytridiomycosis.

The study, published in the journal Biological Conservation, shows that the fungus is also a threat to terrestrial-breeding amphibians with important ecological functions, which include controlling insects that transmit diseases such as dengue, yellow fever, and zika.

“The fungus attacks the amphibian’s skin, which is where it exchanges gas with the external environment. Infection causes a physiological imbalance, and the animal eventually dies from a heart attack,” said Diego Moura-Campos, first author of the article. The study was conducted during his master’s research at the University of Campinas’s Institute of Biology (IB-UNICAMP) in the state of São Paulo, with a scholarship from the Brazilian Ministry of Education’s Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES).

The investigation was conducted under the aegis of the project Chytrid fungus in Brazil: origin and consequences, linked to the FAPESP Research Program on Biodiversity Characterization, Conservation, Restoration and Sustainable Use (BIOTA-FAPESP) and coordinated by Luís Felipe Toledo, a professor at IB-UNICAMP and a co-author of the article.

“We’ve studied the fungus from several angles, but have rarely had the unhappy opportunity to see animals dying from fungal infection in the wild. This is the first study to show the phenomenon in Brazil. If an amphibian dies and is infected, that doesn’t mean the fungus caused its death. It might be coexisting with the pathogen without developing the disease. In this case, we were sure it was the cause of death because the animals had the right symptoms, such as weight loss, heavily sloughing skin, and very high infection loads,” said Toledo, who is also principal investigator for another project that focuses on understanding how the fungus spreads in nature.

The researchers believe direct-developing species (which reproduce on land and lack a tadpole, with terrestrial eggs hatching as fully formed miniature adults) are even less adapted to the fungus. Aquatic species have been in contact with the pathogen for longer and may have developed a degree of resistance to infection.

Moura-Campos observed morbidity and mortality in infected frogs during a field survey conducted on the Serra do Japi Biological Reserve in Jundiaí, São Paulo, between May 2018 and May 2019. Curiously, dead and dying individuals of the species Brachycephalus rotenbergae were found after an atypical period of drought.

“These animals are very small and hard to find. After dying, they decompose quickly. Finding nine of them dead or heavily diseased in a short period, as we did, suggests others probably died as well,” said Guilherme Becker, a professor at the University of Alabama in the United States and last author of the article.

According to Becker, who is also a visiting professor at UNICAMP under its Graduate Program in Ecology, the study shows that accelerating global climate change in the coming decades will increase the frequency of this type of disease, with causative agents that may become more virulent as hybrids emerge, as already shown in an earlier study by the group

“Lack of soil moisture in the forest where they live may have led these animals to seek hydration in streams and become more contaminated than normal by the fungus,” he said.

Another hypothesis raised by the researchers is that periods of drought may compromise the frogs’ immune system so that they become more vulnerable to the fungus.

Cosmopolitan pathogen

The fungus originated in Asia and has probably spread around the world as a result of the trade in frog meat. Species consumed by humans for this purpose, such as the American bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana), are resistant to the fungus and can be bearers without being infected.

According to a paper published in 2018 in the journal Science with Toledo as a co-author, the fungus originated on the Korean peninsula and spread to other parts of the world in the early twentieth century.

Another study to which Toledo contributed also found that the fungus has caused a decline in the populations of at least 501 species of amphibians worldwide. In Brazil alone, at least 50 species or populations have been affected, 12 have become extinct, and 38 have undergone decline (more at: agencia.fapesp.br/30127/). 

“Amphibians are very important to the functioning of many ecosystems. Their biomass in forests is enormous. They serve as food for a wide array of other animals, eat arthropods in the wild, and control communities of invertebrates,” Becker said. “In the case of aquatic species, most are herbivorous in the tadpole stage and consume phytoplankton, which could overwhelm aquatic environments if it were not for tadpoles. These animals cross aquatic and terrestrial ecosystems, so when outbreaks of chytridiomycosis occur, the impact is significant.”

To exemplify, Becker recalled a recent study in which scientists affiliated with institutions in the US and Panama show that amphibian population collapse due to infection by B. dendrobatidis was linked to an increase in outbreaks of malaria in the 1990s and 2000s in Panama and Costa Rica.

According to Becker, Toledo and collaborators, more observation is required over a period of years to reach a more precise estimate of the global impact of chytridiomycosis on amphibian populations.

###

About São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP)

The São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP) is a public institution with the mission of supporting scientific research in all fields of knowledge by awarding scholarships, fellowships and grants to investigators linked with higher education and research institutions in the State of São Paulo, Brazil. FAPESP is aware that the very best research can only be done by working with the best researchers internationally. Therefore, it has established partnerships with funding agencies, higher education, private companies, and research organizations in other countries known for the quality of their research and has been encouraging scientists funded by its grants to further develop their international collaboration. You can learn more about FAPESP at http://www.fapesp.br/en and visit FAPESP news agency at http://www.agencia.fapesp.br/en to keep updated with the latest scientific breakthroughs FAPESP helps achieve through its many programs, awards and research centers. You may also subscribe to FAPESP news agency at http://agencia.fapesp.br/subscribe.

 

Research inspects planetary nebula NGC 6905 and its central star

Research inspects planetary nebula NGC 6905 and its central star
NOT ALFOSC color-composite image of NGC 6905. Credit: Gómez-González et al., 2021.

Using the Nordic Optical Telescope (NOT), astronomers have investigated a planetary nebula known as NGC 6905 and its central star. Results of the study, presented in a paper published October 18 on the arXiv pre-print server, provide more insights into the nature of this object.

Planetary nebulae (PNe) are expanding shells of gas and dust that have been ejected from a star during the process of its evolution from a main sequence star into a red giant or white dwarf. They are relatively rare, but are important for astronomers studying the chemical evolution of stars and galaxies.

At a distance of about 8,800 light years away from the Earth, NGC 6905, also known as the "Blue Flash Nebula" for its characteristic colors, is a high-excitation PN with a clearly clumpy morphology. It is composed of a central roundish cavity with an angular radius of some 0.81  and a pair of extended V-shaped structures extending towards two opposite directions. The central star of this PN, designated HD 193949, is a Wolf-Rayet-type star with a radius of about 0.15 solar radii, mass of approximately 0.6 solar masses, and effective temperature in the range of 150,000–165,000 degrees K.

A team of astronomers led by Víctor Mauricio Alfonso Gómez-González of the National Autonomous University of Mexico has recently conducted a multi-wavelength study of NGC 6905 and HD 193949, aiming to shed more light on the properties and structure of this object. The research is based mainly on the data from NOT's Alhambra Faint Object Spectrograph and Camera (ALFOSC), but also on archival infrared images obtained from telescopes such as NASA's Spitzer and WISE.

"We present a multi-wavelength characterisation of the  (PN) NGC 6905 and its [Wolf-Rayet]-type ([WR]) central star (CSPN) HD 193949. Our Nordic Optical Telescope (NOT) Alhambra Faint Object Spectrograph and Camera (ALFOSC) spectra and images unveil in unprecedented detail the high-ionization structure of NGC 6905," the researchers wrote in the paper.

The observations allowed the team to detect the three broad WR bumps, the so-called O-bump, blue bump and red bump, confirming that HD 193949 belongs to the [WO]-class of Wolf-Rayet . They also detected 21 WR features which suggest that the spectral type of this CSPN cannot be later than a [WO2]-subtype star. The  of HD 193949 was measured to be around 140,000 degrees K, therefore lower than previously thought.

Based on the data, the astronomers investigated the physical properties and chemical abundances of different regions of NGC 6905. They found that the low-ionization knots located at the northwest and southeast regions of this PN do not exhibit different electron density nor electron temperature compared to its other regions. The averaged value of electron density was calculated to be 500/cm3, while the electron temperature was estimated to be at a level of 13,000 degrees K.

The researchers noted that NGC 6905 has similar abundances as other WRPN, but a slightly smaller nitrogen to oxygen ratio.

"In particular, comparing the N/O ratio versus the N abundance following previous studies suggests that the CSPN of NGC 6905 had a relatively low initial mass of about 1 solar mass. This makes NGC 6905 one of the WRPN with the less massive central star," the scientists explained.

The study also allowed the astronomers to conclude that there is no anomalous carbon-enrichment within NGC 6905 which suggests that no very late thermal pulse (VLTP) has been involved in its formation of or the production of its . Additionally, the team reproduced the nebular and dust properties of NGC 6905 and found that the total mass of gas in this PN is in the range of 0.31 and 0.47 solar masses, while the mass of dust was estimated to be between 0.00224 and 0.00169 solar masses.

New giant exoplanet detected with TESS

More information: V. M. A. Gómez-González et al, Planetary nebulae with Wolf-Rayet-type central stars—III. A detailed view of NGC 6905 and its central star. arXiv:2110.09551v1 [astro-ph.SR], arxiv.org/abs/2110.09551

© 2021 Science X Network

 

Fossil dental exams reveal how tusks first evolved

Fossil dental exams reveal how tusks first evolved
Life reconstruction of the the dicynodont Dicynodon. Aside from the tusks in the upper jaw, 
most dicynodonts possessed a turtle-like beak that they used to chew their food.
 Image by Marlene Hill Donnelly. Credit: Marlene Hill Donnelly

A wide variety of animals have tusks, from elephants and walruses to five-pound, guinea pig-looking critters called hyraxes. But one thing tusked animals have in common is that they're all mammals—there are no known fish, reptiles, or birds with tusks. In a new study in Proceedings of the Royal Society B, paleontologists traced the first tusks back to ancient mammal relatives that lived before the dinosaurs, and to do so, they had to define what makes a tusk a tusk in the first place.

"Tusks are this very famous anatomy, but until I started working on this study, I never really thought about how tusks are restricted to mammals," says Megan Whitney, a researcher at Harvard University and the lead author of the study.

"We were able to show that the first tusks belonged to animals that came before modern mammals, called dicynodonts," says Ken Angielczyk, a curator at Chicago's Field Museum and an author of the paper. "They're very weird animals."

The dicynodonts mostly lived before the time of the dinosaurs, from about 270 to 201 million years ago, and they ranged from rat-sized to elephant-sized. Modern mammals are their closest living relatives, but they looked more reptilian, with turtle-like beaks. And since their discovery 176 years ago, one of their defining features has been the pair of protruding tusks in their upper jaws. The name dicynodont even means "two ."

The researchers got the idea to study the origin of tusks while taking a lunch break on a paleontological dig. "We were sitting in the field in Zambia, and there were dicynodont  everywhere," recalls Whitney. "I remember Ken picking them up and asking how come they were called tusks, because they had features that tusks don't have."

Angielczyk had hit upon a crucial distinction: not all protruding teeth are technically tusks, and the teeth's makeup and growth patterns tell us whether they count. "For this paper, we had to define a tusk, because it's a surprisingly ambiguous term," says Whitney. The researchers decided that for a tooth to be a tusk, it has to extend out past the mouth, it has to keep growing throughout the animal's life, and unlike most mammals' teeth (including ours), tusks' surfaces are made of dentine rather than hard enamel.

Fossil dental exams reveal how tusks first evolved
Left side of the skull of the dicynodont Dolichuranus (NMT RB554) from Tanzania. The large tusk is visible at the lower left of the specimen. Photo by K. Angielczyk. Credit: Ken Angielczyk

Under these parameters, elephants, walruses, warthogs, and hyraxes all have tusks. Other big teeth in the animal kingdom don't make the cut, though. For instance, rodent teeth, even though they sometimes stick out and are ever-growing, have an enamel band on the front of the tooth, so they don't count.

Some of the dicynodont tusks that the team observed in Zambia didn't seem to fit the definition of a tusk either— they were coated in enamel instead of dentine.

The different makeup of teeth versus tusks also gives scientists insights into an animal's life. "Enamel-coated teeth are a different evolutionary strategy than dentine-coated tusks, it's a trade-off," says Whitney. Enamel teeth are tougher than dentine, but because of the geometry of how teeth grow in the jaw, if you want teeth that keep growing throughout your life, you can't have a complete enamel covering.

Animals like humans made an evolutionary investment in durable but hard-to-fix teeth— once our adult teeth grow in, we're out of luck if they get broken. Tusks are less durable than our enamel-coated teeth, but they grow continuously, even if they get damaged. It's like the compromise of getting a car that's very reliable but very difficult to get repaired when it does have trouble, versus driving a beater that needs frequent repairs but is a model that's cheap and easy for any mechanic to fix.

The different kinds of teeth animals have evolved can tell scientists about the pressures those animals faced that could have produced those teeth. Animals with tusks might use them for fighting or for rooting in the ground, exposing them to little injuries that would be risky for enamel teeth that don't grow continuously.

To study whether dicynodonts tusks really were tusks, the researchers cut paper-thin slices out of the fossilized teeth of 19 dicynodont specimens, representing ten different species, and examined their structure with a microscope. They also used micro-CT scans to examine how the teeth were attached to the skull, and whether their roots showed evidence of continuous growth. The scientists found that some dicynodont teeth are indeed tusks, while others, particularly those of some of the earlier species, were just large teeth. It wasn't a strict progression from non-tusks to tusks, though— different members of the dicynodont family evolved tusks independently.

Fossil dental exams reveal how tusks first evolved
Isolated tusk fragments found in Zambia by field teams in 2018. Photo by K. Angielczyk. Credit: Ken Angielczyk

Whitney says she was surprised by the finding. "I kind of expected there to be one point in the family tree where all the dicynodonts started having tusks, so I thought it was pretty shocking that we actually see tusks evolve convergently," she says.

"Dicynodont tusks can tell us a lot about mammalian tusk evolution in general," says Angielczyk. "For instance, this study shows that reduced rates of tooth replacement and a flexible ligament attaching the tooth to the jaw are needed for true tusks to evolve. It all ladders up to giving us a better understanding of the tusks we see in mammals today."

"Dicynodonts were the most abundant and diverse vertebrates on land just before dinosaur times, and they're famous for their 'tusks.' The fact that in reality only a few have true tusks, and the rest have big teeth, is a beautiful example of evolution we can document. We can see how to build a tusk!" says Brandon Peecook, a curator at the Idaho Museum of Natural History and one of the paper's authors.

The researchers say that the study, which shows the earliest known instance of true tusks, could help scientists better understand how evolution works.

"Tusks have evolved a number of times, which makes you wonder how—and why? We now have good data on the anatomical changes that needed to happen for dicynodonts to evolve tusks. For other groups, like warthogs or walruses, the jury is still out," says Christian Sidor, a curator at the University of Washington Burke Museum and one of the paper's authors.

"Despite being extremely weird animals, there are some things about dicynodonts, like the evolution of , that inform us about the mammals around us today," says Angielczyk. "Plus, anytime you can say mammals aren't that special, dicynodonts did it first, that's a good day."Thailand seizes large elephant tusks worth over $450,000

More information: The evolution of the synapsid tusk: insights from dicynodont therapsid tusk histology, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences (2021). DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2021.1670. rspb.royalsocietypublishing.or … .1098/rspb.2021.1670

Journal information: Proceedings of the Royal Society B 

Provided by Field Museum 

 

How does 'normal' Internet browsing look today? Now we know

browser security
Credit: Unsplash/CC0 Public Domain

It's 7:15 am on a Friday morning, and Jordan wants to download an application to their laptop. They know the app by name, or so they think; they open a new tab in their Internet browser and mistype the app's name. The error brings them to a malicious website that looks like a legitimate site, only it isn't, causing Jordan to download an app containing malware. Jordan's computer is now infected with malware.

Jordan is a real person, although their name isn't really Jordan. They were a participant in a new study by CyLab researchers that aimed to learn what "normal" Internet browsing looks like. Such datasets didn't previously exist, but now that one does, researchers can better understand how people like "Jordan" are led to download malicious content and come up with ways to prevent that from happening again.

Their study, titled "How Do Home Computer Users Browse the Web?" was published in the latest issue of ACM Transactions on the Web.

"The goal for this paper was to be a foundation that other researchers could use," says CyLab's Kyle Crichton, a Ph.D. student in Engineering and Public Policy and the study's lead author. "Now that we know what normal  looks like, we can start to identify anomalous behavior and begin to address any number of security challenges."

To create their dataset, the authors of the study observed the browsing behavior of 257 willing participants through the Security Behavior Observatory (SBO), a group of participants consenting to have their daily computing behaviors observed. One might think consenting to being monitored may lead one to act a bit different than they normally would, but Crichton says he doesn't believe that happened here.

"In general, there was a substantial number of visits to potentially pirated streaming websites, pornographic websites, and gambling websites," Crichton says. "Therefore, we assume that they were generally behaving as they normally do."

So what does "normal" browsing look like? Lots of browser tab usage—some use just a few and some use a ton—and most time is spent on the top 1% of websites.

"People spend most of their time on a small number of websites," says Crichton. "Fifty percent of people's browsing time is spent on roughly 30 websites, among millions of websites."

Occasionally, Crichton says, people end up at what he refers to as "the periphery" of the Internet—relatively low traffic websites that are commonly associated with riskier content. These sites are often adware, gambling, pornography, and potentially illegal streaming websites.

"We observed a lot of people who started out at a popular streaming service like Netflix or Hulu, and they must not have found what they wanted, then they'd jump out to the periphery," Crichton says.

While the study may serve as a foundation for other researchers to use, it'll do so only until people's browsing behavior evolves enough to necessitate recording a new baseline, which Crichton says is inevitable.

"When Google came out in the late 90s, people's way of finding content quickly changed," he says. "People's browsing behavior shifted again when tabbed browsing was introduced in the mid-2000s. It's these game changers that are introduced, and things rapidly evolve.Misconceptions plague security and privacy tools

More information: Kyle Crichton et al, How Do Home Computer Users Browse the Web?, ACM Transactions on the Web (2021). DOI: 10.1145/3473343

Provided by Carnegie Mellon University 

 

Action video games make players better learners of visual and memory tasks

gaming
Credit: CC0 Public Domain

Playing video games that are heavy on action can make you better at some new tasks. New research reveals that these games are helping by teaching players to be quicker learners.

"Imagine taking an American and putting them through physical training to boost their athleticism," says C. Shawn Green, a psychology professor at the University of Wisconsin-Madison who studies how people learn. "If you then have them try to play rugby for the first time ever, they might not look that good. After all, even a good athlete who is stepping into a rugby match for the first time will have to learn a lot of new rules. However, their increased athleticism will mean that they'll tend to be in slightly better positions initially than people with lesser athleticism, and thus will learn to play rugby more quickly."

That idea is similar for action video games—typically those in a genre called first-person shooters—in which players are rewarded for swiftly and accurately tracking and reacting to features of the game that appear and move quickly.

"If you're increasing the equivalent of athleticism for perceptual —like visual attention or speed of processing—that should allow you to learn faster when you've got a new  that calls on those abilities," Green says.

The results will help researchers understand how gaming—which is used to train laparoscopic surgeons and drone pilots, and to help people with amblyopia (sometimes called "lazy eye") and attention deficit disorders—creates some of its well-documented positive effects.

"Games are really powerful, complex experiences," says Green, whose work is supported by the Office of Naval Research. "We know they produce interesting changes in behavior, but their level of complexity makes them hard to study."

The study also shows that not all training activities are equal. Some types of practice can make you perfect, but only at one thing. Types of training that make trainees better at quickly learning to perform a broad range of tasks—or at least more than one—have obvious advantages.

"If you have people who are new to a basketball court shoot nothing but free throws over and over and over, they'll probably get much better at shooting straight on from 15 feet," Green says. "But then if you have them shoot from somewhere else on the court, they're probably going to go right back to where they started. We call that a failure of transfer or a failure of generalization."

In a pair of xperiments described recently in the journal Communications Biology, 25 participants at the University of Rochester in New York and then 52 participants at the University of Geneva in Switzerland were separated into roughly equal groups assigned to play 45 hours of either action video games (such as those from the Call of Duty series) or other popular video games that unfold at a different pace without relying so much on visual attention and reaction speed (games such as Sims and Zoo Tycoon).

Before the players began their gaming assignments, they were tested with tasks that measured their visual perception and working . The  tasks required participants to use a brief glance to pick out the direction of movement of an object or orientation of stripes running across a shape. The working memory tests were more challenging, asking players to listen and watch for pairs of letters read aloud and shapes appearing in different locations on a screen, and report when one matched a sound or placement from a certain number of turns back.

Both groups came out relatively even in the initial tests. But after their contrasting gaming experiences, the action game players were different.

"They had a slight advantage right away, after playing the action games. But the bigger effect was that they improved faster at these orientation and memory tasks than the people who played other games," says Green, who collaborated on the study with researchers from Rochester, Geneva, New York University and the University of California, Irvine.

The tests were chosen because the simple movements and orientation of basic shapes engage parts of the brain involved in very rudimentary visual processing and working memory. "Constantly having to manage new stuff versus old stuff as information comes in" is a common factor in tackling new tasks, Green says.

Teasing the significant aspects out of the complexity will help future  designers who are focused on training as much as—or more than—entertainment.

"There are issues with action games—they tend to be violent, for example, and it's unlikely that is necessary to cause the effects we want to see in players," Green says. "Before you can start designing games with the goal of maximizing the benefits, you need to know what's helping and what's not.

Playing action video games can boost learning, new study reports


More information: Ru-Yuan Zhang et al, Action video game play facilitates "learning to learn", Communications Biology (2021). DOI: 10.1038/s42003-021-02652-7
Journal information: Communications Biology 

 

Using overpasses as shelter from tornado?

Using overpasses as shelter from tornado?
The wind tunnel and scaled model of the overpass. Credit: Alex Ensign

Meteorologists and emergency workers continue to contest the popular thinking that waiting out a tornado under an overpass is safe. According to the National Weather Service, doing so could actually increase the risk of death, in part because the wind from a tornado is thought to accelerate as it flows under the overpass, in what's known as the wind tunnel effect.

However, few experimental studies show exactly how this acceleration takes  or if it takes place at all. In Physics of Fluids researchers from the University of Rhode Island found no evidence of such acceleration.

"In our research, there is no one finding that would suggest one should or should not use an overpass for protection from a tornado as a last-resort shelter area," co-author D.M.L. Meyer said.

The researchers examined the pressure, velocity, and force fields of tornado-strength winds surrounding an overpass. Experiments were performed in a standard wind tunnel using a scaled geometry of an overpass. A large fan was used to draw air through the tunnel at a top speed of about 130 miles per hour.

The researchers found no wind tunnel effect in their experiments.

"However, that doesn't mean the effect doesn't take place at all, just not in the locations we focused on," Meyer said. "More data and analyses are needed to determine how complex tornado-strength winds interact with the environment underneath an overpass, and our paper provides a start."

An overpass may be a dangerous place for shelter regardless. Wind may not decelerate, depending on the location, and flying debris may reach areas of the overpass that appear to be protected from the .

The researchers studied four locations: immediately above the overpass, immediately below it, between the I-beams, and the center of the travel lane under the overpass. Velocity and dynamic pressure measurements were obtained independently at each location as the overpass was rotated 0-90 degrees at 10-degree increments about its vertical axis. Lift and drag forces were also measured.

Wind tunneling can be explained using a garden hose. Water traveling through the hose flows at a constant flowrate, because the diameter is constant throughout the hose length. Place a thumb partially over the hose opening, and the flow constricts, accelerating the water due to the smaller cross-section. This causes the water to spurt further than without the thumb.

Hurricane straps keep roofs on houses and can improve safety during tornadoes

More information: D.M.L. Meyer et al, Tornado-strength winds interacting with a highway overpass, Physics of Fluids (2021). DOI: 10.1063/5.0065233
Journal information: Physics of Fluids 

 

Study shows medicinal cannabis products can help with depression and improve quality of life

Study shows medicinal cannabis products can help with depression and improve quality of life
Figure 1. Cannabis Users (n = 368) had reduced depression, but not anxiety, relative to Controls (n = 170) on the HADS at baseline. A greater proportion of Cannabis Users also scored below the HADS cutoff for clinical concern (scores ≥ 8) relative to Controls. Scores ranging from 8 to 10, 11 to 14, and 15 to 21 represent approximate cutoffs for mild, moderate, and severe cases, respectively (83). ***p < 0.001. Credit: DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.729800

A team of researchers affiliated with multiple institutions in the U.S. has found evidence that medicinal cannabis products relieve depression and improve the quality of life. In their paper published in the journal Frontiers in Psychiatry, the group describes their study involving online surveys about the benefits of cannabis products.

Prior research findings regarding the use of cannabis-based products to treat depression and anxiety have been mixed. Some patients have shown improvements while others have not. Prior research has also found that other drugs developed to treat depression and anxiety also have mixed results—some have seen improvements while others have not, and some patients have found that they cannot tolerate the side effects of the drugs. Also, some people have begun using medicinally approved  to help with their depression, anxiety,  or sleep disorder, regardless of the research track record—either independently, or with assistance from a doctor. In this new effort, the researchers conducted a  to gauge the opinions of people who use such products, rather than focusing on reports from the  regarding how well they thought they worked.

The study was carried out over four years. At the onset, the researchers received responses from 368 people who reported using cannabis products to reduce their depression and/or anxiety, or to help them sleep. As a , they also received responses from 170 people who were not using cannabis products but who were considering doing so due to their problems with depression or anxiety.

The researchers found that those people taking cannabis products (mostly those containing CBD rather than THC) reported lower levels of depression than the control group, although they saw no difference in anxiety. They also found those taking cannabis products reported a higher quality of life and better sleep than the control group. The researchers also found reduced levels of  later on in those who continued to take cannabis products and also in those who began using them during the study. And interestingly, they also found that those who began taking the products during the study also reported improvements in anxiety and quality of life.

Many adults with breast cancer use cannabis but don't tell their doctors
More information: Erin L. Martin et al, Antidepressant and Anxiolytic Effects of Medicinal Cannabis Use in an Observational Trial, Frontiers in Psychiatry (2021). DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.729800

© 2021 Science X Network

 

What causes disease outbreaks?

What causes disease outbreaks?
Credit: CDC

Since 1974, contaminated water has been the most common driver of large-scale zoonotic infectious disease outbreaks, according to new research from the Center for the Ecology of Infectious Diseases (CEID) at the University of Georgia. The next two greatest drivers are unusual weather patterns and changes in the abundance of disease vectors such as mosquitos and ticks.

Zoonotic diseases occur when pathogens are transmitted from animals to humans—prominent recent outbreaks include Ebola hemorrhagic fever and COVID-19. However, most zoonotic disease outbreaks involve fewer than 100 cases and are quickly brought under control. So what factors drive large-scale outbreaks?

In a study published in the Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, researchers investigated over 4,400 zoonotic infectious disease outbreaks. They identified the 100 largest in terms of numbers of human cases, all of which infected thousands to hundreds of thousands of people. From the full list they also selected 200 outbreaks at random to serve as "case controls." The majority of these control outbreaks included 43 or fewer cases. They then compared the characteristics of large-scale and control outbreaks, particularly how the primary causes of infection and disease spread varied between them.

Lead author Patrick Stephens, an associate research scientist with the Odum School of Ecology and CEID, said that this research is one of the first peer-reviewed studies to quantify the variation of infectious disease outbreak drivers across the globe.

"In the age of COVID-19, it is understandable that many people may not realize how many outbreaks of other infectious diseases are caused by complex, intertwined ecological and socioeconomic conditions," he said. "We know that factors like exposure to wild mammals, habitat disruption, international trade and travel and contact with contaminated food and water are important considerations. Our research was designed to understand what proportion of outbreaks various drivers contributed to. To our knowledge, this study is the first to do so for a global sample of outbreaks of many diseases."

Stephens worked with CEID researchers Nicole Gottdenker of the College of Veterinary Medicine and John Drake, Annakate Schatz and John Paul Schmidt of the Odum School to compile a list of contemporary zoonotic infectious diseases documented in peer reviewed scientific literature. They identified and scored 48 specific infectious disease drivers related to the large-scale and control outbreaks in ecological, environmental and socioeconomic categories.

Water contamination is a key driver of disease outbreaks

Both large-scale and control outbreaks were associated with water contamination, which was the most common driver of large outbreaks and the second most common driver of the smaller control outbreaks. Examples of these water-associated diseases include hepatitis E, typhoid and shigellosis (dysentery). The remaining drivers differed, however.

In addition to , large outbreaks were most often associated with unusual weather patterns, changes in the abundance of vectors—carriers of disease like mosquitos or ticks—and sewage management.

Large outbreaks were also much more likely to be caused by viral pathogens such as SARS coronavirus, influenza virus and Japanese encephalitis virus than were smaller outbreaks. The typical smaller outbreaks were associated with food contamination, local livestock production and human-animal contact. Finally, individual large outbreaks tended to be driven by a greater variety of factors than control outbreaks.

"There is still a lot of work to do to understand how large-scale infectious disease outbreaks can be avoided and controlled," Stephens said. "Perhaps two-thirds of future infectious  outbreaks are expected to be caused by zoonotic pathogens, and the number of these diseases is growing worldwide. Our research is an extremely important first step to better understand global variation in the drivers of outbreaks."

CDC IDs outbreak trends tied to treated recreational water

More information: Patrick R. Stephens et al, Characteristics of the 100 largest modern zoonotic disease outbreaks, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences (2021). DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0535
Provided by University of Georgia