Thursday, April 25, 2019



APRIL 25, 2019

Gestures and visual animations
reveal cognitive origins of linguistic meaning

by New York University

Credit: CC0 Public Domain

Gestures and visual animations can help reveal the cognitive origins of meaning, indicating that our minds can assign a linguistic structure to new informational content "on the fly"—even if it is not linguistic in nature.


These conclusions stem from two studies, one in linguistics and the other in experimental psychology, appearing in Natural Language & Linguistic Theory and Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS).

"These results suggest that far less is encoded in words than was originally thought," explains Philippe Schlenker, a senior researcher at Institut Jean-Nicod within France's National Center for Scientific Research (CNRS) and a Global Distinguished Professor at New York University, who wrote the first study and co-authored the second. "Rather, our mind has a 'meaning engine' that can apply to linguistic and non-linguistic material alike.

"Taken together, these findings provide new insights into the cognitive origins of linguistic meaning."

Contemporary linguistics has established that language conveys information through a highly articulated typology of inferences. For instance, I have a dog asserts that I own a dog, but it also suggests (or "implicates") that I have no more than one: the hearer assumes that if I had two dogs, I would have said so (as I have two dogs is more informative).

Unlike asserted content, implicated content isn't targeted by negation. I don't have a dog thus means that I don't have any dog, not that I don't have exactly one dog. There are further inferential types characterized by further properties: the sentence I spoil my dog still conveys that I have a dog, but now this is neither asserted nor implicated; rather, it is "presupposed"—i.e. taken for granted in the conversation. Unlike asserted and implicated information, presuppositions are preserved in negative statements, and thus I don't spoil my dog still presupposes that I have a dog.

A fundamental question of contemporary linguistics is: Which of these inferences come from arbitrary properties of words stored in our mental dictionary and which result from general, productive processes?

In the Natural Language & Linguistic Theory work and the PNASstudy, written by Lyn Tieu of Australia's Western Sydney University, Schlenker, and CNRS's Emmanuel Chemla, the authors argue that nearly all inferential types result from general, and possibly non-linguistic, processes.


Their conclusion is based on an understudied type of sentence containing gestures that replace normal words. For instance, in the sentence You should UNSCREW-BULB, the capitalized expression encodes a gesture of unscrewing a bulb from the ceiling. While the gesture may be seen for the first time (and thus couldn't be stored in our mental dictionary), it is understood due to its visual content.

This makes it possible to test how its informational content (i.e. unscrewing a bulb that's on the ceiling) is divided on the fly among the typology of inferences. In this case, the unscrewing action is asserted, but the presence of a bulb on the ceiling is presupposed, as shown by the fact that the negation (You shouldn't UNSCREW-BULB) preserves this information. By systematically investigating such gestures, the Natural Language & Linguistic Theory study reaches a ground-breaking conclusion: nearly all inferential types (eight in total) can be generated on the fly, suggesting that all are due to productive processes.

The PNAS study investigates four of these inferential types with experimental methods, confirming the results of the linguistic study. But it also goes one step further by replacing the gestures with visual animations embedded in written texts, thus answering two new questions: First, can the results be reproduced for visual stimuli that subjects cannot possibly have seen in a linguistic context, given that people routinely speak with gestures but not with visual animations? Second, can entirely non-linguistic material be structured by the same processes?

Both answers are positive.

In a series of experiments, approximately 100 subjects watched videos of sentences in which some words were replaced either by gestures or by visual animations. They were asked how strongly they derived various inferences that are the hallmarks of different inferential types (for instance, inferences derived in the presence of negation). The subjects' judgments displayed the characteristic signature of four classic inferential types (including presuppositions and implicated content) in gestures but also in visual animations: the informational content of these non-standard expressions was, as expected, divided on the fly by the experiments' subjects among well-established slots of the inferential typology.


Explore furtherChimp communication gestures found to follow human linguistics rules
More information: Lyn Tieu et al, Linguistic inferences without words, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2019). DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1821018116
Journal information: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences


Provided by New York University

SAFE HEARING PPE

Agronomy Research 12(3), 895–906, 2014


Exposure to high or low frequency noise at workplaces: differences between assessment, health complaints and implementation of adequate personal protective equipment (PPE)


K.Reinhold*, S.Kalle and J.Paju Institute of Business Administration, Tallinn University of Technology, Ehitajate tee 5, EE12618 Tallinn, Estonia

Abstract

Employees are exposed to high and low frequency noise which may cause different health effects. Hearing loss first occurs in the high frequency range, low frequency usually causes sleeping disturbances and annoyance. TES 1358 sound analyzer with 1/3 octave band was used to measure the equivalent sound pressure level, the peak sound pressure level, and the noise frequency spectrum at different workplaces. All the results were compared to Estonian and International legislations. High frequency noise was studied in metal, electronics and wood processing industries. The results showed that in several cases, the normative values were exceeded and the highest values appeared in the range of speech frequencies. Frequency analysis indicated that the noise level spectra at work stations of various machines differed in patterns. The low frequency spectra on a ship showed peaks in the frequency range of 50...1,250 Hz. Most employers provided workers with personal protective equipment against noise, but when selecting ear muffs, noise frequency had not been taken into consideration and therefore workers in the same enterprise used similar ear muffs. Knowledge of the prevailing frequencies assists to decide which ear protection should be used to avoid damage. An adequate hearing protector device can reduce the noise exposure significantly. Key words: Noise, frequency analysis, PPE, occupational hazards. 

INTRODUCTION 

The human perception of sound is between 20...20,000 Hz. The ear is most receptive in the range of 500...8,000 Hz, so called acoustical window, even though the most sensitive range of hearing is 1,000...4,000 Hz (Salvendy, 2012) and the spectrum of human speech ranges in the frequency region of 250...6300 Hz (Cox & Moore, 1988). Health effects from noise exposure have been studied by many researchers. Differences in complaints between low (20...500 Hz) (Alves-Pereira & Castelo Branco, 2007) and high frequency noise have been presented in several sources. Also it has been indicated, that hearing loss tends to occur in the range of high frequencies first (Salvendy, 2012). Industrial noise can mainly be characterized with high frequency noise, but also a considerable number of workers are exposed to low frequency noise on a daily basis. There is a general agreement that progression in hearing loss at frequencies of 500, 1,000, 2,000, and 3,000 Hz eventually will result in impaired hearing, i.e. inability to hear and understand speech (Johnson et al., 2001).



CANADIAN GUIDELINES FOR CONCUSSION




APRIL 25, 2019
Sound of the sea solves decades-old supervolcano mystery

by University of Aberdeen
Solfatara is a shallow volcanic crater located at the centre of Campi Flegrei, 
where volcanic material is emitted through steaming vents. 
Credit: University of Aberdeen

Scientists have used the sound of the sea to discover the route taken by hot fluids that feed a supervolcano in southern Italy.

Using an innovative technique that uses the 'hum' - or seismic noise—of waves crashing at the coastline of Campi Flegrei, scientists have produced a seismic image of the deeper structure of the volcano that reveals the main route bringing hot fluids to the surface.

Their research has featured in a documentary—'The Next Pompeii'—on Nova, a popular science series on major US broadcaster PBS. The documentary highlights the innovative scientific techniques being used to monitor Campi Flegrei – a volcanic caldera to the west of Naples that last erupted five centuries ago.

The area has been relatively quiet since the 1980s, when the injection of volcanic material in the shallower structure of the volcano caused thousands of small earthquakes, which was followed by 38 years of relative silence.

Seismic imaging is one of the main methods used by scientists to accurately map a volcano's structure at depth, however the low level of seismic activity in the area over nearly four decades has meant that Campi Flegrei's inner structure has remained a mystery – until now.

The so-called 'feeder pathway' discovered by scientists is believed to have been formed during the last period of seismic activity in the 1980s, and brings volcanic material from the depths of the volcano, located out at sea.

The material then travels up and along established routes beneath the volcano towards fumaroles at Solfatara and Pisciarelli—located approximately in the centre of the caldera—where they are expelled as vapour through steaming vents.

Seismologists Professor Luca De Siena, Dr. Carmelo Sammarco and Dr. David Cornwell led the study from the School of Geosciences at the University of Aberdeen. They worked alongside the Vesuvius Observatory, which advises the Italian Government's Department of Civil Protection of the threat posed by volcanic activity in the region.

Professor De Siena, now at the University of Mainz, said: "By using the noise at the seashore to create a seismic image, we have finally a better idea of how volcanic material travels from the depths of the volcano to the surface,"

"This is the first time this relatively new technique has been used in a heavily populated area, and it shows us that the feeder pathway created at the beginning of the 1980s appears fully functional in 2011-2013, when we collected the data.

"This is important as it improves our understanding of the character of the volcano, which may ultimately improve monitoring and early warning procedures in an area inhabited by millions of people."


Explore furtherScientists locate potential magma source in Italian supervolcano
More information: L. De Siena et al. Ambient Seismic Noise Image of the Structurally Controlled Heat and Fluid Feeder Pathway at Campi Flegrei Caldera, Geophysical Research Letters (2018). DOI: 10.1029/2018GL078817
Journal information: Geophysical Research Letters


Provided by University of Aberdeen