Thursday, November 30, 2023

 

Climate: why disinformation is so persistent


A UNIGE team has tested six psychological interventions to combat climate misinformation. It shows how difficult it is to combat these messages, which are resistant to scientific information


Peer-Reviewed Publication

UNIVERSITÉ DE GENÈVE




Melting of glaciers, rising sea levels, extreme heat waves: the consequences of climate change are more visible than ever, and the scientific community has confirmed that humans are responsible. Yet studies show that a third of the population still doubts or disputes these facts. The cause is disinformation spread by certain vested interests. To try and prevent this phenomenon, a team from the University of Geneva (UNIGE) has developed and tested six psychological interventions on nearly 7,000 participants from twelve countries. The research, published in the journal Nature Human Behavior, highlights the extremely persuasive nature of disinformation and the need to strengthen our efforts to combat it.


Fighting disinformation about climate change is a major challenge for society. Although scientific consensus on human responsibility - reaffirmed by the sixth report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) - has been in place for decades, a third of the population still doubts or disputes it. This phenomenon can be explained by the disinformation spread by certain companies and lobbies over the last 50 years.


‘‘For instance, these messages can take the form of an unfounded questioning of the scientific consensus or an overestimation of the socio-financial burden of climate policies,’’ explains Tobia Spampatti, a PhD Student and Teaching and Research Assistant in the Consumer Decision and Sustainable Behavior Lab (CDSB Lab) at the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences and at the Swiss Center for Affective Sciences of the UNIGE.


Many psychological factors

This phenomenon weakens the support of a part of the population for climate policies. To combat this, Tobia Spampatti and researchers from the UNIGE developed a theoretical framework to describe the formation and updating of (anti)scientific information. This framework, built on previous theoretical takes on the psychology of misinformation (Philippe Mueller et al. and Ulrich Ecker et al. in 2022), takes into account the source of the message, its content, its recipients, and the psychological factors that can influence their processing. This theoretical framework aims to identify the entry points for disinformation to access a person’s ‘‘psyche’’, and can be used to intervene and block, or encourage, people to accept information.


‘‘As individuals, we do not process scientific messages as neutral receivers of information, but by weighing them up against our prior beliefs, desired outcomes, emotional ties and socio-cultural and ideological backgrounds. Depending on the configuration of these psychological factors, anti-scientific beliefs can be amplified and become resistant to correction,’’ explains Tobia Spampatti, first author of the study.


Six preventive strategies put to the test

On this basis, the researchers developed six psychological intervention strategies aimed at preventing climate disinformation from affecting people’s climate-related beliefs and behaviors. They were tested on 6,816 participants in twelve different countries. Each strategy was linked to a particular theme (scientific consensus, trust in climate scientists, transparent communication, moralizing climate action, accuracy, positive emotions towards climate action). The participants were divided into eight groups: six subjected to one of these strategies, one to disinformation without prevention, and a control group.


The ‘‘trust in climate scientists’’ group, for example, received verified information demonstrating the credibility of IPCC scientists. The “transparent communication” group, meanwhile, was presented with information on both the advantages and the disadvantages of climate mitigation actions. Each group was then exposed to twenty pieces of false or biased information, ten on climate science and ten on climate policy. The UNIGE scientists then measured their impact after these preventive interventions by asking the participants about their feelings regarding climate mitigation actions.


Low preventive effect

‘‘We found that the protective effect of our strategies is small and disappears after the second exposure to disinformation. Climate disinformation used in this study has a negative influence on people’s belief in climate change and their sustainable behaviour’’, says Tobias Brosch, Associate Professor in the CDSB Lab at the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences and at the Swiss Center for Affective Sciences in the UNIGE, and final author of the study. ‘‘Disinformation is therefore extremely persuasive, seemingly more so than scientific information. Only the ‘accuracy’ group, who were asked to think in depth about the accuracy of the information they encountered online, showed a slight advantage’’.


‘‘Research in this field is still in its infancy. We are therefore going to continue our work and look for more effective forms of intervention. It is becoming increasingly urgent to combat this phenomenon, which is delaying the implementation of certain urgent climate change mitigation measures,’’ concludes Tobia Spampatti.

JOURNAL

DOI

METHOD OF RESEARCH

SUBJECT OF RESEARCH

ARTICLE TITLE

ARTICLE PUBLICATION DATE

Psychological science can help counter spread of misinformation, says APA report


Details systemic and individual strategies

Reports and Proceedings

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION




WASHINGTON – Debunking, “prebunking,” nudging and teaching digital literacy are several of the more effective ways to counter misinformation, according to a new report from the American Psychological Association.

Written by a panel of U.S. and international experts on the psychology of misinformation, the report outlines the processes that make people susceptible to misinformation and offers solutions to combat it.

People are more likely to believe misinformation if it comes from groups they belong to or if they judge the source as credible, according to the report “Using Psychological Science to Understand and Fight Health Misinformation: An APA Consensus Statement.” It defines misinformation as “any information that is demonstrably false or otherwise misleading, regardless of its source or intention.”

The report outlines the key features of misinformation that fool people into believing and spreading it. For instance, it found that people are more likely to believe false statements that appeal to emotions such as fear and outrage. They are also more likely to believe misinformation that paints groups that they view as “others” in a negative light. And people are more likely to believe information the more it is repeated, even when it contradicts their prior knowledge. These findings suggest that it is important to stop misinformation early, the report says.

The report also describes features of social media that help misinformation spread very quickly. “Rapid publication and peer-to-peer sharing allow ordinary users to distribute information quickly to large audiences, so misinformation can be policed only after the fact (if at all),” the report says. “’Echo chambers’ bind and isolate online communities with similar views, which aids the spread of falsehoods and impedes the spread of factual corrections.” 

As a result, “most online misinformation originates from a small minority of ‘superspreaders,’ but social media amplifies their reach and influence.”

There are two levels on which misinformation can be stopped, according to the report: systemic approaches, such as legislation and technology standards, and individual approaches focused on changing individual behaviors. The latter include: 

  • fact-checking, or debunking; 
  • prebunking, or pre-emptive debunking to prevent people from falling for misinformation in the first place; 
  • nudges, such as asking people to consider the accuracy of information before sharing it, or rewarding people to be as accurate as possible; 
  • and formal education or community outreach to raise people’s awareness about healthy online behavior and media use.

The report acknowledges that there is much more to learn and recommends more research funding and industry cooperation to understand behaviors related to misinformation and create tools to correct it. The panel members who wrote the report spent more than a year reviewing the scientific literature to develop their recommendations. The report was commissioned by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and funded as part of a $2 million grant to develop effective solutions to COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. 

While the panel’s recommendations focus on health misinformation, they can also be used for broader topics such as politics and climate change. For instance, these findings offer direct input to one of the main issues highlighted in APA’s Health Advisory on Social Media by addressing tactics that can be used to combat misinformation.  

The report recommends eight steps for policymakers, scientists, media and the public to help curb the spread of misinformation and the risks it poses to health, well-being and civic life: 

  1. Avoid repeating misinformation without including a correction.
  2. Collaborate with social media companies to understand and reduce the spread of harmful misinformation.
  3. Use misinformation correction strategies with tools already proven to promote healthy behaviors.
  4. Leverage trusted sources to counter misinformation and provide accurate health information.
  5. Debunk misinformation often and repeatedly using evidence-based methods.
  6. Prebunk misinformation to inoculate susceptible audiences by building skills and resilience from an early age.
  7. Demand data access and transparency from social media companies for scientific research on misinformation.
  8. Fund basic and translational research into the psychology of health misinformation, including ways to counter it.

“These psychological science findings help to explain how misinformation enters our thought processes,” the report states. “It is effortful and difficult for our brains to apply existing knowledge when encountering new information; when new claims are false but sufficiently reasonable, we can learn them as facts. Thus, everyone is susceptible to misinformation to some degree: we acquire it even when we know better.”

The American Psychological Association, in Washington, D.C., is the largest scientific and professional organization representing psychology in the United States. APA’s membership includes over 146,000 researchers, educators, clinicians, consultants and students. Through its divisions in 54 subfields of psychology and affiliations with 60 state, territorial and Canadian provincial associations, APA works to advance the creation, communication and application of psychological knowledge to benefit society and improve people’s lives.


 

Dishing the dirt on human evolution: Why scientific techniques matter in archaeology



Scientists should seek answers hidden in the dirt using proven and state-of-the-art archaeological science techniques to support new discoveries about human evolution following recent controversies at a cave site in Africa


Peer-Reviewed Publication

FLINDERS UNIVERSITY

Resin-casting of blocks permit further sampling and analyses of dirt 

IMAGE: 

RESIN-CASTING OF BLOCKS PERMIT FURTHER SAMPLING AND ANALYSES OF DIRT WITHOUT DISTURBING OR SPOILING THEIR SPATIAL RELATIONSHIPS. (IMAGE CREDIT: DR JULIEN LUBEEK).

view more 

CREDIT: DR JULIEN LUBEEK.




Scientists should seek answers hidden in the dirt using proven and state-of-the-art archaeological science techniques to support new discoveries about human evolution following recent controversies at a cave site in Africa, says a group of international experts.

Their recommendations follow claims published in June of this year that Homo naledi—a small-brained human species—buried their dead in Rising Star Cave, South Africa, between 335,000 and 241,000 years ago, and may also have decorated the cave walls with engravings. These claims were accompanied by a Netflix documentary, ‘Unknown: Cave of Bones’.

In scientific commentary published today in the journal Nature Ecology and Evolution, led by Associate Professor Mike Morley at Flinders University (Australia), a group of archaeological scientists strongly advocate for the use of modern scientific techniques to support claims such as those made about Homo naledi.

“Many scientists—including the authors of our comment out today—remain unconvinced by the evidence provided in three papers published online prior to peer review,” says Associate Professor. Mike Morley, Director of the Flinders Microarchaeology Laboratory.

“Fortunately, there are a range of state-of-the-art techniques that scientists can use to study the many forms of evidence for human evolution – the fossils, artefacts and even the sediments (or dirt) from which they have been recovered. These techniques are crucial when attempting to identify features such as the burials they claim to have found at Rising Star Cave”

Dr Ania Kotarba, a co-author from the University of Adelaide, says that the routine use of these techniques to generate supporting data will help avoid future controversies and increase the confidence that the public hold in such claims.

“When you consider that significant discoveries are normally small and scarce, ranging in size from teeth to strands of DNA found in caves, then proper scientific analysis holds immense potential to rewrite what we know about the evolution of our closest ancestors,” says Dr Kotarba.

The experts argue that one key scientific technique that is gaining traction in the field is known as micromorphology.

This technique involves the microscopic analysis of sediment that surrounds fossils or archaeology. By studying intact blocks of sediment removed from archaeological trenches, microscopic clues can be pieced together to reconstruct the past environments present at the site and in the local environment.

“Micromorphology has proven to be a powerful tool for analysing ancient human remains and burial practices. In 2021, scientists who studied the oldest known human burial (78,000 years ago) published in the journal Nature used micromorphology to help identify the burial” says Vito Hernandez, a PhD candidate using this technique in his research at Flinders University.

“Developments in analytical techniques mean that archaeological science can provide information about the past at the molecular and even elemental scales. Through these developments we now better understand the processes that form sites and preserve fossils and artefacts in incredibly detailed ways.”

In their commentary, the researchers agree that they would like to see a future where all archaeological investigations use scientific techniques such as these from the outset as standard to avoid future controversy.

“In that way we might avoid future controversy and find clues that strongly support hypotheses and allow for greater confidence in findings that are presented to the scientific community and public alike,” says Associate Professor. Mike Morley.

“This is especially important in the ‘post-truth’ world that we now live in where it is incredibly difficult for the public to disentangle facts from fiction. Scientists need to be much more careful about how they communicate their findings to avoid increasing scepticism towards scientists that can have a major impact across all aspects of modern life.

An intact block of dirt which holds evidence of animal and human occupations in Denisova cave in Siberia, a famous human evolution site from where the first Denisovan genome was sequenced.

Flinders University graduate student in geoarchaeology, Kelsey Hamilton, microexcavates an intact block of dirt encased in plaster wrapping.

Members of the Flinders Microarchaeology Laboratory microsampling dirt for further geochemical and DNA analysis.

CREDIT

Associate Professor Mike Morley, Flinders University.

 

Durable plastic pollution easily, cleanly degrades with new catalyst


Fishing nets, carpet, clothing break down without leaving harmful byproducts behind


Peer-Reviewed Publication

NORTHWESTERN UNIVERSITY

Fishing net 

IMAGE: 

A SEA TURTLE ENTANGLED IN AN ABANDONED FISHING NET.

view more 

CREDIT: NOAA





Many people are familiar with the haunting images of wildlife — including sea turtles, dolphins and seals — tangled in abandoned fishing nets.

The main issue behind Nylon-6, the plastic inside these nets, carpet and clothing, is that it’s too strong and durable to break down on its own. So, once it’s in the environment, it lingers for thousands of years, littering waterways, breaking corals and strangling birds and sea life.

Now, Northwestern University chemists have developed a new catalyst that quickly, cleanly and completely breaks down Nylon-6 in a matter of minutes — without generating harmful byproducts. Even better: The process does not require toxic solvents, expensive materials or extreme conditions, making it practical for everyday applications.

Not only could this new catalyst play an important role in environmental remediation, it also could perform the first step in upcycling Nylon-6 wastes into higher-value products.

The research will be published on Thursday (Nov. 30) in the journal Chem.

“The whole world is aware of the plastic problem,” said Northwestern’s Tobin Marks, the study’s senior author. “Plastic is a part of our society; we use so much of it. But the problem is: What do we do when we’re finished with it? Ideally, we wouldn’t burn it or put it into landfills. We would recycle it. We’re developing catalysts that deconstruct these polymers, returning them to their original form, so they can be reused.”

Marks is the Charles E. and Emma H. Morrison Professor of Chemistry and Vladimir N. Ipatieff Professor of Catalytic Chemistry at Northwestern’s Weinberg College of Arts and Sciences and a professor of materials science and engineering at Northwestern’s McCormick School of Engineering. He also is a faculty affiliate at the Paula M. Trienens Institute for Sustainability and Energy. Northwestern co-authors include Linda J. Broadbelt, the Sarah Rebecca Roland Professor of Chemical and Biological Engineering and senior associate dean of McCormick, and Yosi Kratish, a research assistant professor in Marks’ group.

A deadly difficulty

From clothing to carpet to seat belts, Nylon-6 is found in a variety of materials that most people use every day. But, when people are done with these materials, they end up in landfills or worse: loose in the environment, including the ocean. According to the World Wildlife Federation, up to 1 million pounds of fishing gear is abandoned in the ocean each year, with fishing nets composed of Nylon-6 making up at least 46% of the Great Pacific Garbage Patch.

“Fishing nets lose quality after a couple years of use,” said Liwei Ye, the paper’s lead first author who is a postdoctoral fellow in Marks’ laboratory. “They become so water-logged that it’s difficult to pull them out of the ocean. And they are so cheap to replace that people just leave them in the water and buy new ones.”

“There is a lot of garbage in the ocean,” Marks added. “Cardboard and food waste biodegrades. Metals sink to the bottom. Then we are left with the plastics.”

The greenest solvent is no solvent

Current methods to dispose of Nylon-6 are limited to simply burying it in landfills. When Nylon-6 is burned, it emits toxic pollutants such as nitrogen oxides, which are linked to various health complications including premature death, or carbon dioxide, an infamously potent greenhouse gas.

Although other laboratories have explored catalysts to degrade Nylon-6, those catalysts require extreme conditions (such as temperatures as high as 350 degrees Celsius), high-pressure steam (which is energetically expensive and inefficient) and/or toxic solvents that only contribute to more pollution.

“You can dissolve plastics in acid, but then you are left with dirty water,” Marks said. “What do you do with that? The goal is always to use a green solvent. And what type of solvent is greener than no solvent at all?”

Recovering building blocks for upcycling

To bypass these issues, the researchers looked to a novel catalyst already developed in Marks’ laboratory. The catalyst harnesses yttrium (an inexpensive Earth-abundant metal) and lanthanide ions. When the team heated Nylon-6 samples to melting temperatures and applied the catalyst without a solvent, the plastic fell apart — reverting to its original building blocks without leaving byproducts behind.

“You can think of a polymer like a necklace or a string of pearls,” Marks explained. “In this analogy, each pearl is a monomer. These monomers are the building blocks. We devised a way to break down the necklace but recover those pearls.”

In experiments, Marks and his team were able to recover 99% of plastics’ original monomers. In principle, those monomers then could be upcycled into higher-value products, which are currently in high demand for their strength and durability.

“Recycled nylon is actually worth more money than regular nylon,” Marks said. “Many high-end fashion brands use recycled nylon in clothes.”

Efficiently targeting Nylon-6

In addition to recovering a high yield of monomers, the catalyst is highly selective — acting only on the Nylon-6 polymers without disrupting surrounding materials. This means industry could apply the catalyst to large volumes of unsorted waste and selectively target Nylon-6.

“If you don’t have a catalyst that’s selective, then how do you separate the nylon from the rest of waste?” Marks said. “You would need to hire humans to sort through all the waste to remove the nylon. That’s enormously expensive and inefficient. But if the catalyst only degrades the nylon and leaves everything else behind, that’s incredibly efficient.”

Recycling these monomers also avoids the need to produce more plastics from scratch.

“These monomers are produced from crude oil, so they have a huge carbon footprint,” Ye said. “That’s just not sustainable.”

What’s next?

After filing a patent for the new process, Marks and his team have already received interest from potential industrial partners. They hope others can use their catalysts on a large scale to help solve the global plastic problem.

“Our research represents a significant step forward in the field of polymer recycling and sustainable materials management,” Ye said. “The innovative approach addresses a critical gap in current recycling technologies, offering a practical and efficient solution for the nylon waste problem. We believe it has implications for reducing the environmental footprint of plastics and contributing to a circular economy.”

The study, “Catalyst metal-ligand design for rapid, selective and solventless depolymerization of Nylon-6 plastics,” was supported by RePLACE (Redesigning Polymers to Leverage A Circular Economy), funded by the Office of Science of the U.S. Department of Energy (award numbers SC0022290 and DE-FG02-03ER15457) and the National Science Foundation (grant number CHE-1856619). Additional support came from the Institute for Catalysis in Energy Processes, which is a major research project within the Center for Catalysis and Surface Science at the Paula M. Trienens Institute for Sustainability and Energy.

New catalyst degrades Nylon-6 sample within minutes

New process recovers 99% of monomers (shown here), nylon's building blocks. After recovering monomers, industry can upcycle nylon into higher-value products.


Catalyst in action [VIDEO] |

Watch the catalyst work to degrade a 1 gram sample of Nylon-6.

CREDIT

Northwestern University


No sex please - marine life turned off by swimming in plastic chemicals

Plastic waste in the water might be stopping - or interrupting - some shrimp-like creatures from reproducing


Peer-Reviewed Publication

UNIVERSITY OF PORTSMOUTH

Mating behaviour of shrimp like creatures exposed to plastic chemicals 

VIDEO: 

MATING BEHAVIOUR OF SHRIMP LIKE CREATURES EXPOSED TO PLASTIC CHEMICALS

view more 

CREDIT: UNIVERSITY OF PORTSMOUTH




Plastic waste in the water might be stopping - or interrupting - some shrimp-like creatures from reproducing.

In a unique study, the ability of ‘shrimp like’ creatures to reproduce successfully was found to be compromised by chemicals found in everyday plastics.  

Research showed that little critters, known as marine amphipod Echinogammarus marinus, changed their mating behaviour when exposed to toxic plastic additives.

Until now, most research into plastic pollution has focused on visual plastics; what can get trapped in plastics and the dangers of ingesting large particles.  Scientists from the University of Portsmouth have taken a different approach and investigated the chemicals that are used as ingredients in plastics.

Professor Alex Ford, from the Institute of Marine Sciences at the University of Portsmouth, says: “This unsuccessful mating behaviour has serious repercussions, not only for the species being tested but potentially for the population as a whole. These animals form pairs to reproduce. Once they were exposed to a chemical, they would break apart from their mate and take much longer -in some cases days - to repair, and sometimes not at all. 

“These creatures are commonly found on European shores, where they make up a substantial amount of the diet of fish and birds. If they are compromised it will have an effect on the whole food chain.”

There are over 350,000 chemicals in use around the world in everyday products. Ten thousand of these are used to enhance plastics. Chemicals can be used to make plastics more flexible, add colour, give sun protection or make plastic flameproof. Around one third of these chemicals are known to be toxic to human’s immune, nervous or reproductive systems.

The study, published in the journal Environmental Pollution, tested four widely used chemicals found in plastics. These plastic additives are used in a variety of common products, for example, phthalates (DEHP and DBP) which are found in medical supplies, food packaging and toys. Triphenyl phosphate (TPHP) is mainly used as a flame retardant in products like nail polish and electronic equipment, including cables, and N-butyl benzenesulfonamide (NBBS) is used in nylon, medical devices, cooking utensils and films.

Bidemi Green-Ojo, lead author and PhD Researcher in Environmental Toxicology at the University of Portsmouth, says: “We chose these four additives because the suspected danger they pose to human health is well documented. Two of the chemicals we investigated (DHP and DEHP) are regulated and not allowed to be used in products in Europe. The other two chemicals have no current restrictions on them and are found in many household products. We wanted to test the effects these chemicals had on aquatic mating behaviour.”

The ‘shrimp like’ creatures which have been studied are known to pair up and typically lock together for two days while mating. Pairs of them were exposed to each chemical, and researchers monitored their behaviour over four days, measuring the time it took for the creatures to mate. They found that at best it took much longer for the creatures to re-pair, and at worst they didn’t re-pair. A video of the experiment shows the problem.  

The experiment found that all the plastic additives had the capacity to reduce the overall percentage of animals that formed pairs. The ones which did form pairs took longer to make contact and re-pair.

Two of the chemicals caused a concentration-dependent effect on shrimps’ sperm, resulting in a decline of up to 60 per cent in sperm count of those exposed to elevated levels of the chemicals.

“Although the animals we tested were exposed to much higher concentrations than you would normally find in the environment, the results indicate these chemicals can affect sperm count”, explains Professor Ford.

“It is conceivable that if we did the experiment on shrimps that had been exposed for a longer period or during critical stages in their life history, it would affect their sperm levels and quality.”

Bidemi Green-Ojo adds: “We must understand more about these chemicals and how they affect behaviour. Many types of behaviour - such as feeding, fight or flight mode, and reproduction - are essential in an animal's life, and any abnormal behaviour may reduce the chances of survival.

“We are urging environment agencies around the world to take more notice of behavioural data, because sometimes the data tells us things that normal toxicity tests don’t. Studies like this give a different perspective on potential damage caused by a specific pollutant.”

Mating behaviour of shrimp like creatures having been exposed to plastic chemicals.

Bidemi Green-Ojo, lead author and PhD Researcher in Environmental Toxicology at the University of Portsmouth (IMAGE)

UNIVERSITY OF PORTSMOUTH

CREDIT

JOURNAL

DOI

METHOD OF RESEARCH

SUBJECT OF RESEARCH

ARTICLE TITLE