Wednesday, October 09, 2024

 

Why people think they’re right, even when they are wrong



Study finds people may incorrectly believe they have all the facts



Ohio State University



COLUMBUS, Ohio – If you smugly believe you’re right in a disagreement with a friend or colleague, a new study suggests why you may actually be wrong.

 

Researchers found that people naturally assume they have all the information they need to make a decision or support their position, even when they do not.

The researchers called it the “illusion of information adequacy.”

“We found that, in general, people don’t stop to think whether there might be more information that would help them make a more informed decision,” said study co-author Angus Fletcher, a professor of English at The Ohio State University and member of the university’s Project Narrative.

“If you give people a few pieces of information that seems to line up, most will say ‘that sounds about right’ and go with that.”

The study was published today in the journal PLOS ONE. Fletcher completed the work with co-authors Hunter Gehlbach, an educational psychologist at Johns Hopkins University’s School of Education, and Carly Robinson, a senior researcher at Stanford University’s Graduate School of Education

The study involved 1,261 Americans who participated online. 

They were split into three groups who read an article about a fictional school that lacked adequate water. One group read an article that only gave reasons why the school should merge with another that had adequate water; a second group’s article only gave reasons for staying separate and hoping for other solutions; and the third control group read all the arguments for the schools merging and for staying separate.

The findings showed that the two groups who read only half the story – either just the pro-merging or the just the anti-merging arguments – still believed they had enough information to make a good decision, Fletcher said.  Most of them said they would follow the recommendations in the article they read.

“Those with only half the information were actually more confident in their decision to merge or remain separate than those who had the complete story,” Fletcher said.

“They were quite sure that their decision was the right one, even though they didn’t have all the information.”

In addition, participants who had half the information said that they thought that most other people would make the same decision they did.

There was one piece of good news from the study, Fletcher said. Some of the participants who had read only one side of the story later read the arguments for the other side. And many of those participants were willing to change their minds about their decision, once they had all the facts.

That may not work all the time, especially on entrenched ideological issues, he said.  In those cases, people may not trust new information, or they may try to reframe it to fit their preexisting views.

“But most interpersonal conflicts aren’t about ideology. They are just misunderstandings in the course of daily life,” Fletcher said.

These findings offer a complement to research on what is called naïve realism, the belief people have that their subjective understanding of a situation is the objective truth, Fletcher explained.  Research on naïve realism often focuses on how people have different understandings of the same situation.

But the illusion of information adequacy shows that people may share the same understanding – if they both have enough information.

Fletcher, who studies how people are influenced by the power of stories, said people should make sure they have the full story about a situation before they take a stand or make a decision.

“As we found in this study, there’s this default mode in which people think they know all the relevant facts, even if they don’t,” he said.

“Your first move when you disagree with someone should be to think, ‘Is there something that I’m missing that would help me see their perspective and understand their position better?’ That’s the way to fight this illusion of information adequacy.”

 

Predatory birds from the same fossil formation as SUE the T. rex



Unusual foot bones suggest that the newly-discovered species may have hunted like modern hawks and owls



Peer-Reviewed Publication

Field Museum

Fossil foot bones 

image: 

The three fossil specimens (from left to right), Avisaurus darwini, Avisaurus sp., and Magnusavis ekalakaensis, all of which are represented by a tarsometatarsus. They are all shown to scale with one another. 

view more 

Credit: Alex Clark




The Hell Creek Formation in what’s now the Dakotas, Montana, and Wyoming was once home to some of the world’s most beloved dinosaurs, like Triceratops and Tyrannosaurus rex (including SUE, one of the largest, most complete, and best-preserved T. rex specimens ever found). But these giant dinosaurs weren’t alone in their ecosystem, and in a paper in the journal PLOS ONE, scientists have described two new species of birds that lived alongside these dinosaurs 68 million years ago. The researchers were able to name these new species from just one bone each: the powerful foot bone that suggests these birds could have captured and carried off prey.

“Based on clues in their foot bones, we think these birds would have been able to catch and carry prey, similar to what a modern hawk or owl does,” says Alex Clark, a PhD student at the Field Museum and the University of Chicago and the study’s lead author. “While they might not be the first birds of prey to ever evolve, their fossils are the earliest known examples of predatory birds.”

The three fossils Clark studied in this paper had been collected in the past several years by researchers at other institutions, but there hadn’t been much work done on them. Clark recalls that when he first saw the fossils, they weren’t especially dazzling-- they were all a foot bone that the toes attach to, called the tarsometatarsus, and they’d been found on their own, without other, flashier body parts like skulls and claws. And while the bones were large for bird tarsometatarsi, they were still only about the size of an adult human thumb.  However, these isolated bones proved to be a treasure trove of information.

“Every nook and cranny and bump that occurs on a bone can tell us something about where the muscles or tendons attached and how big they were,” says Clark. On these bones, there was an especially noteworthy bump-- a muscle attachment point called a tubercle. On each bone, it was larger and farther down than in most birds. “When we see tubercles this large and this far down in modern birds, they're in birds of prey like owls and hawks,” says Clark. “That’s because when they hunt and pick up their prey with their feet, they're lifting proportionally heavy things and holding them close to their bodies to stay as aerodynamically efficient as possible. These fossil ankle bones look like they're built to do something similar.”

Clark and his colleagues conducted a series of biomechanical analyses comparing the fossil foot bones to those of a variety of modern birds. “The muscles and bone of the ankle work like a lever, and by comparing how far down on the bone the muscle attaches, we can get a good idea of how it would have moved and how strong it would have been,” says Clark. The math corroborated the researchers’ hypothesis that these feet would have been strong enough for these hawk-sized  birds to pick up small mammals and even baby dinosaurs.

From the three foot bones, Clark and his team described two new species to science: Avisaurus darwini, after Charles Darwin, and Magnusavis ekalakaenis, in honor of the town of Ekalaka, Montana, where the fossil was found. (The third bone may be another new species, but the fossil’s degraded condition made it difficult to tell for sure.) All of these birds are part of a group called the avisaurids. They belong to a larger group of birds called the enantiornithines, which went extinct with most of their fellow dinosaurs when the asteroid hit 66 million years ago.

“These discoveries have effectively doubled the number of bird species known from the Hell Creek Formation and will be critical for helping us to better understand why only some birds survived the mass extinction that wiped out T. rex and the avisaurids described here,” Jingmai O’Connor, the Field Museum’s associate curator of fossil reptiles in the Negaunee Integrative Research Center, Clark’s advisor, and a co-author of the paper.

“​​'I’m really proud and very impressed with what Alex was able to do with these specimens. They're each just a single bone. But he brings his background as an ecologist into his paleontological work to tell more than the average paleontologist about what an animal’s life would have been like,” says O’Connor. “Alex has done a superb job with being able to extract so much incredible ecological information from just a single bone.”

An illustration of the newly described Avisaurus darwini, whose unusual foot bones indicate that it was one of the earliest birds of prey known to science, shown here carrying a small mammal.

Credit

Illustration by Ville Sinkkonen



Lead author Alex Clark with the fossil foot bone of Avisaurus darwini, with SUE the T. rex, a fellow Hell Creek fossil, in the background.

Credit

Photo by Kate Golembiewski, Field Museum.

 

Sexist textbooks? Review of over 1200 English-language textbooks from 34 countries reveals persistent pattern of stereotypical gender roles and under-representation of female characters across countries


PLOS
Sexist textbooks: Automated analysis of gender bias in 1,255 books from 34 countries 

image: 

Gender biases around male and female roles and under-representation of female characters appeared in textbooks from around the world.

view more 

Credit: Jessica Ruscello, Unsplash, CC0 (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/)





Gender biases around male and female roles and under-representation of female characters appeared in textbooks from around the world, with male-coded words appearing twice as often as female-coded words on average, according to a study published October 9, 2024 in the open-access journal PLOS ONE by Lee Crawfurd from the Center for Global Development, United Kingdom, and colleagues.

School textbooks play an important role in shaping norms and attitudes in students—one reason why controversy over textbook content is high in many countries today. In this study, Crawfurd and colleagues investigated how gender norms are depicted in textbooks around the world.

The authors used a particularly large corpus of textbooks to conduct their analysis: 1,255 publicly available online English-language school textbooks spanning subjects and grade levels from grades 4-13 from 34 countries downloaded over 2020-2022. They compared textbook content with predefined lists of gendered nouns and pronouns (e.g. “Auntie/she/her/woman”) and investigated how often these gendered words were associated with key words used in previous studies relating to achievement, appearance, family, home, and work (e.g. “powerful/gorgeous/household/executive”) within the textbook. Finally, the authors compared their text analysis results with other measures of gender equality at the country level.

They found that on average across the full sample of textbooks, there were more than twice as many occurrences of male words (178,142) as female words (82,113), though there was considerable variation between countries. After adjusting for book length, grade, and subject, the countries with the lowest representation of women and girls were Afghanistan, Pakistan, Sri Lanka, and South Sudan, where fewer than 1 in 3 gendered words were female.

Across all countries, the adjectives most likely to describe only female and not male characters included “married”, “beautiful”, “aged”, and “quiet”. Verbs for only female characters included “bake”, “cook,” and “sang”. The adjectives most likely to describe male and not female characters included “powerful”, “rich”, “wise”, “certain”, and “unable”. Verbs for only male characters included “rule”, “guide”, “sign”, and “order”. Almost all of the individual achievement- and work-themed words showed a stronger association with male words than female words, and the individual appearance- and home-themed words showed a stronger association with female words than male words. The authors note that countries with textbooks containing a greater number of female characters also had stronger GDPs and more legal rights for women compared to countries with less female representation, though this is only correlation and cannot speak to causation.

The authors also note there are several limitations to this work—their tool was not able to assess non-text items (such as images) and was not always correct at parsing names (though the authors used manual validation where possible), and the analysis reflects a binary view of gender illustrated in the textbooks. Furthermore, this analysis is restricted to English language literature and therefore may not be generalizable to languages beyond English. However, the results suggest that combating gender biases in textbooks could potentially lead to real-world effects.

The authors add: “Our findings reveal a troubling reality: school books are perpetuating outdated gender stereotypes. Schools should broaden horizons not limit children's potential. It's crucial for policymakers and educators to address these disparities.”

#####

Author Interview: https://plos.io/4dybR17

In your coverage please use this URL to provide access to the freely available article in PLOS ONEhttps://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0310366

Citation: Crawfurd L, Saintis-Miller C, Todd R (2024) Sexist textbooks: Automated analysis of gender bias in 1,255 books from 34 countries. PLoS ONE 19(10): e0310366. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310366

Author Countries: U.K., U.S.A.

Funding: This research was supported by funding from the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation and Echidna Giving. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.