Friday, July 05, 2024

 

How our brain decodes other people’s gaze



A UNIGE team has succeeded in determining the exact moment when the brain detects another person’s gaze direction.


UNIVERSITÉ DE GENÈVE





The gaze plays a central role in everyday social interactions. Our capacity for instant communication relies on the brain’s ability to detect and interpret the direction of others’ gaze. How does our brain detect gaze direction, and what factors influence the process? In a recent study published in the journal NeuroImage, a team from the University of Geneva (UNIGE) succeeded in determining with unprecedented precision the exact moment at which the direction of gaze is detected. These findings significantly enhance our understanding of autism spectrum disorders and could offer therapeutic prospects for people affected by Alzheimer’s disease. 


Human faces are the most common and consistent visual stimuli that we encounter from the second we are born. Our brain has developed the expertise to memorize and recognize faces, as well as to interpret messages they convey. For instance, the direct eye gaze signals a desire to engage in social interaction, while avoiding eye contact conveys the opposite message. But how rapidly can our brain comprehend the gaze of others? This topic has been extensively researched. However, existing publications predominantly focus on studying the eye region in isolation, neglecting other factors like head orientation.


Cerebral analysis of gaze

A team from UNIGE presented to study participants the 3D avatars, each featuring different head and gaze directions. In the first task, volunteers were asked to indicate the orientation of the head, while in the second task, they had to identify the direction of the eyes. By analysing the brain activity using an electroencephalogram, the research team has discovered that these two processes can be reliably decoded independently of each other.


‘‘The experiment also demonstrates a certain hierarchy in the processing of these two information. The brain first perceives the more global visual cues, i.e. the orientation of the head, from 20 milliseconds onwards, before focusing on the more local information, i.e. the eyes, from 140 milliseconds onwards. This hierarchical organisation then allows for integration of eye region and head orientation information, to ensure the accurate and effective judgement of gaze direction,’’ explains Domilė Tautvydaitė, a postdoctoral fellow and associate researcher at the UNIGE, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, and the study’s first author.


The study also demonstrates that the decoding of gaze direction was significantly more accurate when participants were specifically asked to pay attention to the gaze of the presented faces. This means that the task context influences the perception and understanding of the gaze. ‘‘In everyday life, these results show that when people are actively engaged in a ‘social mode’, they are better and faster at recognizing the intentions of other people,’’ explains Nicolas Burra, senior lecturer at the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences and director of the Experimental Social Cognition Laboratory (ESClab) at UNIGE, who led this research.


A cutting-edge method

The method used provides extremely accurate results for these two mechanisms. By integrating the analysis of neural activity using electroencephalography (EEG) with machine-learning techniques, the research team could predict the decoding of gaze and head direction even before the participants were aware of it. ‘‘This method represents a significant technical innovation in the field, allowing for a much more precise analysis than it was previously attainable,’’ adds Nicolas Burra.


In people with autism spectrum disorders, the decoding of this information may be impaired, and the avoidance of eye contact may be preferred. This is also the case for Alzheimer’s disease, where during disease’s evolution, memory difficulties impoverish the person’s relationships with others and often lead to social withdrawal. It is therefore essential to understand the neural mechanisms in detecting the gaze direction.


The study results and the method used make a concrete contribution to the early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders in children. Concerning Alzheimer’s disease, one of the most striking symptoms as the disease progresses is the inability to recognise faces, even those of family members. This study therefore paves the way for a better understanding of the neural mechanisms linked to reduced social interaction and memory for faces- a subject currently being studied by Dr Tautvydaitė at McGill University in Canada. The UNIGE’s ESClab laboratory research will continue in this field by analysing these processes during real-life social interactions.

No comments: